Activities per year
Abstract
Over the last decades, economic inequality has escalated across the world
(Piketty, 2020). For instance, in the US, the wealthiest 0.1% own almost as
much wealth as the bottom 90% of American families (Saez & Zucman, 2019). This
concentration of capital has detrimental consequences; it reduces social
mobility and economic growth, worsens health and educational outcomes, erodes
trust and increases polarization (see Lobeck & Støstad, 2023). Reducing
economic inequality is thus “a pivotal challenge for modern democracies”
(Lobeck & Støstad, 2023, p. 2). One effective way to reduce extreme wealth
inequalities is redistributive policies (Saez & Zucman, 2019). Yet, support
for such policies tends to be low (Kuziemko et al., 2015) with preferences
strongly influenced by perceptions of inequality (Gimpelson & Treisman, 2018)
and political attitudes (Alesina et al., 2018). To aid in understanding this
issue, Walker et al. (2021) in PNAS recently provided evidence that drawing
individuals’ attention towards a wealthy individual (vs. group) lowers
support for redistribution by leading people to judge the individual’s
wealth as well-deserved, highlighting a strong “person-positivity” bias.
However, do people understand the true extent of economic inequality and might
the observed bias differ across political ideologies? We address this in three
preregistered conceptual replications of Study 5 in Walker et al. (2021) with
US nationally representative samples (N = 4,308), where we investigate how
graphical and scaled representations of wealth inequality influence fairness
perceptions, attributions of wealth accumulation, and support for
redistribution among different political orientations. In study 1,
participants (n = 1462) were randomly assigned to one of four conditions: the
original written descriptions of individual or group wealth, or new conditions
where these descriptions were accompanied by graphical representations scaled
to a US median income, $1 million, and $1 billion. We replicated the
person-positivity bias in terms of attributions (d = .31) and fairness (d =
.45), but further found that graphical and scaled depictions of wealth
inequality reduced this bias, making extreme wealth appear less fair (d = .35)
and more due to situational factors (d = .25). However, support for
redistribution did not significantly differ between any of our conditions.
Study 2 aimed at increasing the comparability between conditions by using one
scaling point of median household wealth across all conditions. Participants
(n = 1479) were assigned to one of four conditions, like Study 1 but with
consistent scaling points, ensuring clearer comparisons between graphical and
written presentations of wealth inequality. We again replicated the
person-positivity bias for fairness judgments (d = .31) and attributions for
wealth accumulation (d = .20), but our results indicated that graphical
depictions could amplify the person-positivity bias in terms of attributions
(d = .34) and fairness judgments (d = 0.50). We again found now significant
differences across conditions in terms of support for redistribution. Study 3
focused solely on isolating the effects of presentation format (written vs.
graphical) and scaling reference (median income vs. wealth). We did this by
only focusing on conditions where inequality was framed around a group (vs.
individual). In this study we introduced new measures of dispositional envy,
compassion, personal gain from redistribution to disentangle individual
differences in the observed outcomes. We found no significant differences in
attributions, perceived fairness or support for redistribution across
conditions. Yet, dispositional envy (b = .55, p < .001), compassion (b = .67,
p < .001), and expected personal gain (b = 1.01, p < .001) were significant
predictors of support for redistribution, while more conservative political
views predicted less support (b = -.22, p < .001), hence replicating previous
work by Sznycer et al. (2017) in PNAS. Finally, our computational analyses
using Model Based Recursive Partitioning revealed notable variations in
support for redistribution across political orientations, depending on how
economic inequality was presented and scaled. For liberals, graphical and
scaled representations tended to increase support for redistribution. For
moderates however, graphical representations often resulted in a decrease in
support for redistribution, suggesting potential adverse effects of such
framing. Finally, we found trends indicating that graphical representations
could sometimes increase, and other times decrease support for redistribution
among this group depending on the scaling point used. Collectively, we
conclude that (1) the person-positivity bias observed by Walker et al. (2021)
is robust, but (2) that graphical and scaled representations of wealth
inequality can influence perceptions of fairness and attributions of wealth
accumulation. However, neither of these effects significantly alter support
for redistributive policies. Our results thus underscore the complexity of
redistributive preferences and suggest that policy framing alone may be
insufficient to shift attitudes. However, our results highlight how wealth
inequality can be effectively communicated to different political groups and
underscore the need for tailored approaches that consider the diverse
psychological and ideological factors.
(Piketty, 2020). For instance, in the US, the wealthiest 0.1% own almost as
much wealth as the bottom 90% of American families (Saez & Zucman, 2019). This
concentration of capital has detrimental consequences; it reduces social
mobility and economic growth, worsens health and educational outcomes, erodes
trust and increases polarization (see Lobeck & Støstad, 2023). Reducing
economic inequality is thus “a pivotal challenge for modern democracies”
(Lobeck & Støstad, 2023, p. 2). One effective way to reduce extreme wealth
inequalities is redistributive policies (Saez & Zucman, 2019). Yet, support
for such policies tends to be low (Kuziemko et al., 2015) with preferences
strongly influenced by perceptions of inequality (Gimpelson & Treisman, 2018)
and political attitudes (Alesina et al., 2018). To aid in understanding this
issue, Walker et al. (2021) in PNAS recently provided evidence that drawing
individuals’ attention towards a wealthy individual (vs. group) lowers
support for redistribution by leading people to judge the individual’s
wealth as well-deserved, highlighting a strong “person-positivity” bias.
However, do people understand the true extent of economic inequality and might
the observed bias differ across political ideologies? We address this in three
preregistered conceptual replications of Study 5 in Walker et al. (2021) with
US nationally representative samples (N = 4,308), where we investigate how
graphical and scaled representations of wealth inequality influence fairness
perceptions, attributions of wealth accumulation, and support for
redistribution among different political orientations. In study 1,
participants (n = 1462) were randomly assigned to one of four conditions: the
original written descriptions of individual or group wealth, or new conditions
where these descriptions were accompanied by graphical representations scaled
to a US median income, $1 million, and $1 billion. We replicated the
person-positivity bias in terms of attributions (d = .31) and fairness (d =
.45), but further found that graphical and scaled depictions of wealth
inequality reduced this bias, making extreme wealth appear less fair (d = .35)
and more due to situational factors (d = .25). However, support for
redistribution did not significantly differ between any of our conditions.
Study 2 aimed at increasing the comparability between conditions by using one
scaling point of median household wealth across all conditions. Participants
(n = 1479) were assigned to one of four conditions, like Study 1 but with
consistent scaling points, ensuring clearer comparisons between graphical and
written presentations of wealth inequality. We again replicated the
person-positivity bias for fairness judgments (d = .31) and attributions for
wealth accumulation (d = .20), but our results indicated that graphical
depictions could amplify the person-positivity bias in terms of attributions
(d = .34) and fairness judgments (d = 0.50). We again found now significant
differences across conditions in terms of support for redistribution. Study 3
focused solely on isolating the effects of presentation format (written vs.
graphical) and scaling reference (median income vs. wealth). We did this by
only focusing on conditions where inequality was framed around a group (vs.
individual). In this study we introduced new measures of dispositional envy,
compassion, personal gain from redistribution to disentangle individual
differences in the observed outcomes. We found no significant differences in
attributions, perceived fairness or support for redistribution across
conditions. Yet, dispositional envy (b = .55, p < .001), compassion (b = .67,
p < .001), and expected personal gain (b = 1.01, p < .001) were significant
predictors of support for redistribution, while more conservative political
views predicted less support (b = -.22, p < .001), hence replicating previous
work by Sznycer et al. (2017) in PNAS. Finally, our computational analyses
using Model Based Recursive Partitioning revealed notable variations in
support for redistribution across political orientations, depending on how
economic inequality was presented and scaled. For liberals, graphical and
scaled representations tended to increase support for redistribution. For
moderates however, graphical representations often resulted in a decrease in
support for redistribution, suggesting potential adverse effects of such
framing. Finally, we found trends indicating that graphical representations
could sometimes increase, and other times decrease support for redistribution
among this group depending on the scaling point used. Collectively, we
conclude that (1) the person-positivity bias observed by Walker et al. (2021)
is robust, but (2) that graphical and scaled representations of wealth
inequality can influence perceptions of fairness and attributions of wealth
accumulation. However, neither of these effects significantly alter support
for redistributive policies. Our results thus underscore the complexity of
redistributive preferences and suggest that policy framing alone may be
insufficient to shift attitudes. However, our results highlight how wealth
inequality can be effectively communicated to different political groups and
underscore the need for tailored approaches that consider the diverse
psychological and ideological factors.
Original language | English |
---|---|
Publication date | 20 Aug 2024 |
Publication status | Accepted/In press - 20 Aug 2024 |
Event | Annual Meeting of the Society for Judgment and Decision Making 2024 - New York Marriott Marquis, New York City, United States Duration: 22 Nov 2024 → 25 Nov 2024 |
Conference
Conference | Annual Meeting of the Society for Judgment and Decision Making 2024 |
---|---|
Location | New York Marriott Marquis |
Country/Territory | United States |
City | New York City |
Period | 22/11/2024 → 25/11/2024 |
Fingerprint
Dive into the research topics of 'The importance of representing economic inequality saliently and to scale'. Together they form a unique fingerprint.Activities
- 1 Participation in or organisation af a conference
-
Annual Meeting of the Society for Judgment and Decision Making 2024
Elbæk, C. T. (Participant)
22 Nov 2024 → 25 Nov 2024Activity: Participating in or organising an event types › Participation in or organisation af a conference